Rupert Wegerif
  • Home
  • About
  • Books
  • Publications
  • Talks & Media
  • Blog

​What is involved in switching perspective?

10/2/2016

1 Comment

 
Picture
What is involved in switching perspective?
Switching perspective or seeing things from a different point of view, is a key ‘mechanism’ in my dialogic theory of education – and probably for any dialogic theory of education - but what exactly does this mean? I do not think it is quite as simple as it may at first seem.
​
1)Point of view in a shared objective space
A popular image of a switch in perspective is to see a landscape from one point of view and then to move to see it from another point of view. Piaget, being Swiss, developed a task with mountains to see when children could see things as if from a dolls point of view placed somewhere else in relation to the landscape. Younger children who could not describe what the doll could see but only what they could see were said to be egocentric. Those who could describe the scene from the point of view of the doll were capable of a more decentred understanding indicative of cognitive development in general.
 

2)Point of view when there are many worlds of experience
Piaget’s experiment assumed a single objective space such that what was seen from each perspective was always the same. This mostly works for seeing from the other side of a mountain on a sunny day but is not strictly true of every perspective switch in a dialogue.

Last year a Twitter war about the colour of a dress, - is it white and gold or blue and black? – drew attention to the fact that people see things differently.

Picture
It seems that neuroscience has found that about 70% of the signals making up our vision are internally generated and do not come from the ‘objective’ outside world. One expert goes so far as to describe perception as guided hallucination (See https://youtu.be/3MSw2irv0-A)
 
If people can even see a publically accessible image of a dress so differently it is not surprising that their understanding of concepts like justice, truth and beauty and so on will be very different. Each person is in fact the centre of their own unique world formed by their own unique history and, indeed, their own unique biology. As the social anthropologist Margaret Mead put it, ‘Always remember that you are absolutely unique. Just like everyone else’.

The idea that there are multiple different worlds of experience or ‘empirical realities’ might sound rather subjective and not very scientific. However, a version of this theory has been the orthodoxy since Einstein’s theory of relativity about one hundred years now. According to Einstein’s theory there is no universal simultaineity or experience of ‘now’. What seems to be ‘now’ is relative to the position and velocity of the observer.

Of course most people most of the time will share enough ‘spacetime’ in common to be able to communicate. Similarly, even though everyone has a unique brain generating apparent reality for them in a unique way,  most of us share enough overlap in our perceptions to be able to communicate. In both cases this is because we do in fact share the same ‘objective reality’, just that this is at the transcendental level of underlying structures revealed by science – the formulas of relativity theory for example - and not necessarily at the empirical level of our experience in which everyone’s ‘now’ is a bit different from everyone else’s.

In this situation where each is the centre of their own unique, if overlapping, world, a switch in perspective is more complex than simply changing position with someone on the other side of a mountain. Some effort needs to be made to re-construct the world as it appears and as it feels from a different perspective, building up from the shared structures that come with our basic physical humanity without being able to assume any shared cultural frame of reference.  
 
3)The basic transcendental/empirical perspective switch

When I first learnt to create simple web-sites using HTML coding language I was delighted to find that simply typing a simple hexadecimal number in my script could change the colour of my whole web-site into any known colour. I tried different codes out in the script, clicked save, then re-loaded to site to see that it had all gone golden or aquamarine or black. On the one hand I was looking at the underlying code that shaped my experience, on the other hand I was enjoying the experience. The two sides, the code and the experience, did not meet. It is not possible for me to see both at the same time. In this kind of perspective switch there is no continuity of experience but a gap. Either you are on one side, viewing the code, or on the other, viewing the experience.

Changing the colour on a web-site might not seem very significant but imagine I was programming immersive virtual reality experiences with cyber googles, cyber gloves and a multidirectional treadmill. I could switch perspective from code on the one side to lived experience on the other.

This is what science does when it moves between the transcendental code, like quantum theory, and the empirical phenomena that are shaped by that code, like ‘observing’ the traces of sub-atomic participles. Weaving between the underlying code and the surface experience is not just a feature of natural science but also of social science. Underlying invisible social structures shape experience and emotion making some things seem obvious and others seem impossible. If we want to improve human life as educators we need to understand the way in which experience and feelings are programmed in order to change the programming for the better.  

In the ‘Thinking Together’ research programme we found that classroom talk was shaped by implicit assumptions or ‘ground rules’ that did not always lead to the best or happiest results. By examining these and re-shaping them through guided educational experience we were able to change the culture of the classroom in ways which led to different experiences. On the whole teachers and students reported happier classrooms where problems were resolved through talking which had previously led to conflict.

4)Reversal of perspectives in a dialogue

Picture
​A 


​
The switch in perspective to see oneself as if through the eyes of another is rather more like the empirical/transcendental switch described above than like imagining what it is like to see from the other side of the mountain. This is because, in a dialogue, the other person is not just another body in a landscape, like the doll in Piaget’s experiment, but she is my outside as I am her outside. Moving between these two perspectives is switching between insides and outsides of experience with no continuity of the kind you might get with camera tracking as you move from seeing the mountain on one side to seeing it on the other. In fact there is a gap between these two perspectives exactly as there is in switching from writing the computer code to experiencing the effect of the computer code.

Looking out from the inside I am the ‘transcendental’ gaze that constructs the other as an image in my field of vision. My invisible categories and underlying assumptions will shape her as an object. Her accent will locate her in a social field. Her clothes, her choice of words, her hair-style etc, all will converge on a representation that sums up who she is. BUT in this case the other is also my outside. She contains me and judges and constructs me as an image in her field of vision. In entering into dialogue with her I immediately surrender part of my sovereign right to construct her and allow her the right to, at least partially, construct me. In order to dialogue I need to be able to ‘incarnate’ her on the inside of me. I need to take her seriously. Even to hear her voice I need to reconstruct her position to make sense of what she is saying and in doing that, in making an effort to actually understand what she is saying to me, I have to allow her to be a transcendental other, someone who constructs meaning as I do, someone who is the centre of her own world, and in that world, in the world she constructs, I find myself located as one amongst others.

5)Dialogue with the outside
​



Picture
Simply through dialogue I find myself involved, whether I like it or not, in looking at myself as if from the outside. An outside that I do not contain but that is transcendental to me, using transcendental here in the traditional sense of ‘going beyond’. The outside perspective contains me but I do not contain it. Most often in dialogues I do not change my point of view simply because of another person’s point of view. I can usually explain away their point of view with an argument on the lines of ‘well she would say that wouldn’t she?’. But the problem is that, in the process of dialogue, I am drawn to see myself as if from the outside and it is this relationship with the outside that is harder to shake off.

Dialogue with the outside, of what I call elsewhere, the Infinite Other, is not symmetrical. This other contains me but I do not contain it. It seems to me to take the form of a horizon around me but I know that it exists also beyond the horizon. When it talks to people I notice that they are often tempted to give it a form. If, for example, it points out that they did something wrong even though no-one else has noticed, they might think of it as the voice of God or an ancestor. When it points out insights that they did not see, that no-one had seen, they might think of it as a like a spirit guide or, in a different context, even like the future community of scientists able to see and understand much more than their limited contemporaries (Habermas invokes this future community). But whatever form the Infinite Other seems to take it is obvious that it is not limited to that form but transcends that form because its essential nature is not to be pinned down.

When I have made this argument in conferences people sometimes say to me that ‘that is just people talking to themselves, it is their voice that they hear’. In other words what people take to be the voice of the outside is really just them. I can see why we want to say that what appears to people as the voice of the outside is not really outside them because physically it comes from within their brains. I am not completely convinced by this assumption of a physical frame locating thought within individual brains as I explain elsewhere (Dialogic: Education for the Internet Age, download pdf and search on neuro) but playing along with it for a minute there is a reason why we need to distinguish our own voice from the voice of the outside. This came out most clearly in an unexpected side-effect in experiments on ‘Aha’ creativity described by Kounios and Beeman. A clever experimental design enabled Kounios and Beeman to track sudden insight solutions to problems as these arose in the brains of participants. Before the insight appeared there was a desire to close the eyes or blink more frequently linked to specific patterns of brain activity indicative of inhibiting some areas. In other words, they conclude, in my interpretation at least, that in order to allow the often weak signals of insights to come through we need to supress the stronger signals associated with the activities of what we take to be ourselves in order to allow voices to speak that appear to us to be ‘other’ [Kounios, J., & Beeman, M. (2009). The Aha! Moment the cognitive neuroscience of insight. Current directions in psychological science, 18(4), 210-216.]
​

Picture
​The fact that these other voices, voices bringing the unexpected solutions to problems in this case, are in some sense ‘in your brain’ is not all that surprising given that, in a sense, everything that there is for you including me, Australia and the Milky Way could be said to be, ‘in your brain’. It is certainly not a good reason to say that when you listen to voice of the Infinite Other you are just listening to yourself unless we also want to say that when you listen to me now, reading this blog post, you are also just listening to yourself. The point is that in order to be able to learn new things you have to be able to switch off your voice and listen to other voices, often quiet, tentative, hard to hear voices that seem to come to you as if from the outside. This is a dialogic skill akin to a switch in perspective in a dialogue. Even though the position of being the outside is not available to you it is nonetheless possible to go at least half-way and be more open to the voice of the other which is also to be more open to the voice of the outside. ​
1 Comment

What is ‘Dialogic Space’?

5/2/2016

2 Comments

 
Dialogic Space is both a practical idea in the classroom and a slippery philosophical notion. I introduced it into research on classroom talk in the 1990’s when it seemed like a useful idea to explain the difference between successful and unsuccessful groups solving problems together. I was working with Neil Mercer at the time and using his three types of talk analysis for categorising groups in classrooms. There were two main ways in which groups were not successful at solving problems. Groups that engaged in ‘Cummulative Talk’ agreed with each other and shared ideas but without challenging each other. This worked for some tasks but did not help them solve reasoning tests together. Groups that engaged in ‘Disputational Talk’ disagreed with each other and competed as individuals to get the right answer but without sharing their reasoning. Groups that engaged in ‘Exploratory Talk’ seemed to really be thinking together. (Mercer, 1995).
 
This ‘types of talk’ analysis referred to more than just the words children used. It was an interpretation of the underlying ground rules or assumptions shaping the talk – the pragmatic ‘implicature’, as Grice called this - which could only be interpreted through a sort of participant observation, however vicarious. I mean by this that you can only really interpret what is going on in a dialogue by projecting yourself into that dialogue as if you were participating. The 'types of talk' reflected different types of identification. In cumulative talk the children identified with the group. ‘We think’ was more important than ‘I think’. But this sense of ‘we’ was monologic. The group had an identity and they did not want to disrupt it by challenging this. If anyone said something that did not fit with the group ethos it tended to be ignored. In disputational talk each child very obviously identified with themselves as an ego. Each wanted to be right and wanted the others to be wrong. So what were the children identifying with in the more successful type of talk which we called ‘Exploratory Talk’? This is where the term ‘dialogic space’ was useful. (Wegerif and Mercer, 1997).
 
As well as using the words and interaction patterns associated with Exploratory Talk, successful groups seem to shift their identification from self or group to the process of dialogue itself. This could be seen in shared pregnant pauses indicative of being at ease with a state of uncertainty, individuals being able to ask for help and to change their mind without embarrassment and being able to take up the unfinished utterances of others and finish them without anyone seeming concerned about issues of ownership. These were all signs that they were really ‘thinking together’ as a group.
 
Later research conducted in Mexico under the leadership of Sylvia Rojas-Drummond taught ‘Thinking Together’ (Dawes, Mercer and Wegerif, 2000) to children and compared the talk that resulted around creative tasks as well as around reasoning tasks (Rojas-Drummond et al, 2008). The talk of the ‘Thinking Together’ children improved in both types of task compared to controls. However the successful creative task talk did not have all the features of Exploratory Talk, lacking the explicit reasoning moves. This suggests that ‘Exploratory Talk’ is a sub-set of the larger type of ‘Dialogic Talk’ which is not so much a type of talk since it is now defined not by talk alone but more fundamentally by an orientation or type of identification. Playful talk can also be dialogic as can dialogues in music in dance and in art. (Wegerif, 2005, 2007, 2013).
 
What is 'dialogic' about this kind of interaction goes beyond individual dispositions and can be summed up as a temporary identification with dialogic space. This is a space defined by openness and a multiplicity of voices. It is the opening of a shared space of possibilities in which the dialogue is more important than ownership of ideas. Even the identities of the participants enter into the dialogue. This is why people can learn from dialogue. To enter into dialogue is to surrender some autonomy and allow the possibility of learning something which is the possibility that you will change. 
 
Although I thought that 'Dialogic Space' was my idea at the time, at least in the context of research on classroom social interaction, the same or similar ideas have been put forward before in philosophy. The philosopher of dialogue, Martin Buber, referred to this as the space of the ‘in-between’ (1956). Bakhtin referred to this space when he wrote of how persuasive words enter into my words and re-arrange them from within, whereas authoritative words might be accepted or rejected but do not lead to real dialogue. Real dialogue, for Bakhtin, was a shared space (‘chronotope’ in his language) within which participating voices entered into a relationship of ‘inter-illumination’, each learning more about themselves and their position as they engaged with understanding the voices of the others (Bakhtin, 1986). But for me the philosopher who has produced the most eloquent evocation of dialogic space is the phenomenologist Maurice Merleau-Ponty. 
 
A discussion is not an exchange or a confrontation of ideas, as if each formed his own, showed them to the others, looked at theirs, and went back to correct them on the basis of his own... . Someone speaks, and the others immediately become no more than certain divergence in relation to his words, and he himself specifies his divergence in relation to them. Whether he speaks loudly or barely whispers, each speaks with all that he is, with his “ideas,” but also with his obsessions, his secret history which the others suddenly lay bare by formulating them as ideas. Life becomes ideas and the ideas return to life, each is caught up in the vortex in which he first committed only measured stakes, each is led on by what he said and the response he received, led on by his own thought of which he is no longer the sole thinker. No one thinks anymore, everyone speaks, all live and gesticulate within Being, as I stir within my landscape, guided by gradients of difference to be observed or to be reduced if I wish to remain here or go yonder.” (Merleau-Ponty, 1968, p. 119).
 
Dialogic theory suggests that we learn not so much by replacing wrong ideas with right ideas, but more often by augmenting existing perspectives with new perspectives which enable us to see further or better or just differently (Wegerif, 2013). Dialogic Space, as a concept, builds on and augments a range of related space-type notions.
 
The first contrast is with physical space. In physical space, as Aristotle pointed out, objects cannot occupy the same position at the time. From this we get the rule ‘a thing is what it is and not another thing’ or ‘A=A’, the principle of identity that underlies classic logic. Dialogic space overlaps with physical spaces (physical spacetime to be more accurate). In a classroom one can note where and when dialogic spaces open and where and when they close. Yet they open up a different kind of space with different rules. Your ideas are also my ideas and vice versa, when the dialogue is flowing. Each idea resonates with others and invokes often distant other voices. Whereas physical space assumes a finished surfaces of things where bodies each have a discrete and unique place, dialogic space undermines this to open up new possibilities through the potential interconnectedness of everything with everything else. Everything physical become a metaphor to think with. The pencil and ruler on the table between us are no longer fixed in meaning but can be used by me to explain my ideas as can my doodles on bits of paper. Physical things become intersubjective signs only within Dialogic Space.
 
The next contrast for me is with conceptual space or the space of reasons (Brandom referred to by Jan Derry 2008). In Mathematics one version of rational space is Hilbert Space in which many mathematical ideas can be mapped in terms of multi-dimensional vectors. Vygotsky mapped out a conceptual space of concepts (1986, p198) as a kind of global grid and perhaps he thought of education as drawing children from savage participatory thinking into this space of ideas. Opening up Dialogic Space brings with it, as one aspect, this space of concepts.  Dialogic Space includes conceptual space and the only way into conceptual space, initially at least, is through dialogue. But, more than this, Dialogic Space is also a space of resonances between bodies, of feelings, of historical and cultural colours and traces. Whereas rational spaces are defined and divided off from other spaces, dialogic space has no preset boundaries. It is not uncommon for ideas to enter into the dialogue unbidden not because of explicit reasoning but because of a resonance in form or other associations that might seem random at first but connect with something in the dialogue. The poetic resonance of words is a feature of Dialogic Space but not of conceptual space (See Damasio, 1994 for a neuro-science account of how and whay ‘reasons’ always inhere within bodily emotional guidance systems).
 
A related, contrast is with design spaces. A design space is the space of possibilities used in combining different elements to create a new product. Any product will have a design space underlying it of shapes, colours, materials and so on that could be combined in different ways. Creative thinking is a kind of design and some have suggested that creative thinking could be modelled by computers using design spaces (Margaret Boden). Like design spaces dialogic space opens a range of new possibilities. However, unlike design spaces, there are no boundaries fixed in advance so no fixed positions or parameters. Dialogic Space is a design space only it is an infinite design space. Think of the ultimate design space, the design space that underlies the Universe, not only the periodic table but also the range of crystals,  animals, processes and so on.  Of course any given Dialogic Space might by framed by the assumptions pertaining in the cultural context of the dialogue. As Merleau-Ponty puts it, each dialogic space has its own inner landscape with mountain peaks, areas of darkness and clearings in the forest. But these landscapes are virtual, open to change, and ultimately opening onto the infinite potential of all things to interrelate to all things which is the metaphysical truth of dialogic space – a kind of deep dialogue that Bakhtin referred to as ‘Great Time’, the chronotope (timespace) in which every voice intercommunicated with every other voice.
 
Observations about Dialogic Space(s)
 
Any given dialogic space can be deepened through a process of ‘de-construction’ that unpicks the key distinctions that frame the dialogue thereby expanding the range of possible thoughts. 
 
A given dialogic space can be widened by bringing in a new and different perspective. When this new perspective is ‘radically’ different this implies also a process of deepening since framing assumptions have to opened up in order to allow the new previously ‘other’ voice into the dialogue.

A dialogue can also be focussed by imposing a framework of differences relevant to the task at hand. However, for creativity, it is often best to combine a specific focus with an unlimited background horizon of possible resonances that could potentially be brought to bear.
 
Dialogic space appears to be many spaces on the surface of empirical space-time but all these spaces have a shared depth in the infinite space of potential meaning which is the ‘transcendental’ depth of dialogue, ie the underlying structure that makes dialogue and the opening of dialogic space possible in the first place (see earlier blog on transcendental). It is as if there were lots of puddles of water on a plain surface and each puddle seemed unique but actually they were all connected by hidden channels on the inside to an underground ocean. This transcendental underlying structure is not really a ‘thing’ or a ‘self’ but is perhaps best described by Simondon’s concept of the ‘pre-individual’, that which is there as a potential for meaning before various divisions enable us to carve out a more precise meaning. Merleau-Ponty refers to a similar idea, in the context of perception, as the ‘pre-thematic’. 

To make sense of Dialogic Space we need a relational ontology (Gergen) or difference ontology (Heidegger and Derrida) as opposed to the more traditional substance ontology. As Merleau-Ponty writes in the quotation above, meaning in dialogue is not about building up blocks of meaning but it is about 'divergence'. Each new claim diverges in a new way and introduces a new structure of the whole. This is like throwing a new pebble in a pond and seeing how the ripples structure the whole surface of the pond interacting with other ripples from other pebbles. Derrida does not use the term ontology but spells out the idea of a difference ontology very clearly when he writes of: 'all the others of physis--tekhnè, nomos, thesis, society, freedom, history, mind, etc.—as physis differed and deferred, or as physis differing and deferring'. In other words things are not self-subsistent substances, they are not really things at all, but they only appear to be things because of a series of differentiations that have been in a prior underlying potential for all meaning which I prefer to call the 'pre-individual'. As Simondon writes, it is because we all participate in the pre-individual that we are able to join up and participate in trans-individual identities. Identification with Dialogic Space is a transindividual identity. This is not an unscientific position, by the way, Simondon was interpreting quantum theory which many think implies a relational ontology and shows the limites of substance ontology. 
.
On the inside the limit of dialogue is self-identity. When my ideas coincide with yours there is no more need for talk. On the outside it is a lack of trust. When my freely expressed ideas are taken and used as evidence against me then the dialogue breaks down. When participants become self-conscious the dialogue breaks down. Then it can seem that the stretch between perspectives was too great. One might think: ‘I assumed a mutual understanding and empathy that did not really exist’. But mutual understanding and empathy can be built through careful scaffolding of activities and experiences together. That is the job of educators in expanding the space of dialogue.
 
When people think creatively and reflectively on their own , then they open a Dialogic space and bring multiple voices to bear on a problem or issue. They learn to do this by first talking with others. Dialogic Space (mind) does not emerge with bodily maturation – it comes to the individual through the culture. It is when he or she is taken over by the culture and taken up into the culture as Dorothy was picked up by a worldwind in the film the Wizard of Oz.
 
Voices in dialogue are not all given equal respect (as some claim). Some voices assume authority because they seem to say what the participants recognise needs to be said. We give authority to other voices because we authorise them to speak on our behalf. This voice then becomes our voice. This authority comes because the voice seems to subtend a greater area of the real, to be a ‘higher perspective’ making more sense of multiple ‘lower’ points of view’. This can be misleading. Authoritative voices have a glow of the infinite ‘outside’ about them. Like the silhouette of someone framed by the light shining through the window.
 
Ultimately the greatest authority belongs to the voice of the Infinite Other. This is the voice of the dialogue itself. It is a silent voice (See Bakhtin’s account of the silence of Christ in his Grand Inquisitor dialogue). The voice of the dialogue understands all but has no specific content. It is the voice that always sees further and so challenges each consensus. There is always an outside. There is no final word.
 
Teacher’s voices tend to have authority within dialogic spaces but they can use that authority to open up possibilities for learners to speak. Learners can ‘find their own voice’ in a dialogue precisely by ‘authorising’ a voice that comes to them and possesses them to speak. Waiting is important and then sometimes you know what to say. When you find yourself saying what needs to be said and not pretending this is someone else’s voice but owning it as your voice, this is when you find your voice. When you find yourself pulled out to speak by the Infinite Other that is when you find yourself at your most individual and also at your most universal.

Of course it is perhaps more common for the Infinite Other (the outside) to call to you through a more culturally situated ‘superaddressee’ voice like the voice of science for example. The voice that responds to the challenge in science and is authorised by you as your voice enables you to find your voice within a field, the specific field of science where you find yourself adding to the discourse. You find your voice as a voice amongst others.
​
The voice that responds to the call of the Infinite Other directly does not stem from this image or that image, your body, your status as a student or professor or whatever, being British or French is irrelevant, the voice that responds is like a pure circuit across the whole of ‘being’, connecting the inside to the outside. The real you, it turns out, is the Infinite Other on the inside.
 
These are just a few thoughts about dialogues and dialogic spaces. I expect that I have it wrong It is not as if I have understood it yet. I am sure that you can add your own observations. Please do.
 
Some references
 
Bakhtin, M. M. (1981). The dialogic Imagination. Four essays by M. M. Bakhtin. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Bakhtin, M. M. (1986). Speech genres and other late essays. Austin: University of Texas.
Buber, M. (1958). I and Thou (Second Edition) (R. Gregory Smith, trans.). Edinburgh: T & T Clark.
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Putnam.
Dawes, L., Mercer, N. and Wegerif, R. (2000) Thinking together: a programme of activities for developing speaking, listening and thinking skills for children aged 8-11. Birmingham: Imaginative Minds Ltd.
Derry, J. (2008). Abstract rationality in education: from Vygotsky to Brandom. Studies in Philosophy and Education, 27(1), 49-62.
Grice, H. P. 1975. Logic and conversation, in Syntax and Semantics, 3: Speech Acts, (P. Cole and J. Morgan, eds). New York: Academic Press.
Mercer, N. (1995). The guided construction of knowledge: Talk amongst teachers and learners. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1968). The Visible and the Invisible (Ed. by C. Lefort and translated by
A. Lingis). Evanston, Il: Northwestern University Press.
Merleau-Ponty, M.(1964). Le Visible Et L'invisible, Paris,Gallimard.
Rojas, S., Drummond, M., Fernandez, N., Mazon, R. & Wegerif, R. (2006). Explicit reasoning, creativity and co-construction in primary school children's collaborative activities. Thinking Skills and Creativity 1(2), 84-94.
Simondon, G. (1989) L'individuation psychique et collective (Paris, Aubier, reprinted in 2007 with a preface by Bernard Stiegler).
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (A. Kozulin, trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wegerif, R. (2005). Reason and creativity in classroom dialogues. Language and Education 19(3), 223-238.
Wegerif, R. (2013). Dialogic: Education for the Internet age, London. Routledge.
Wegerif, R. and Mercer, N. (1997) A Dialogical Framework for Investigating Talk. In Wegerif, R. and Scrimshaw, P. (Eds) Computers and Talk in the Primary Classroom, pp 49-65. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. ISBN: 1853593915
2 Comments

    Author

    Rupert Wegerif. Professor of Education at Cambridge University. Interested in Dialogic Education, educational technology and teaching for thinking and creativity.

    Top posts

    • Dialogic Education
    • Chiasm: dialogic research methodology
    • Religious Education in the Internet Age
    • ​Education for representation or for relationship?
    • Teaching Thinking with the Matrix
    • Dialogic vs Dialectic​
    • Types of talk
    • Groundhog day, Nietzsche and the meaning of life​
    • How to write desk-based research in education​
    • ​Understanding Dialogic Space

    Archives

    August 2022
    April 2022
    November 2021
    August 2021
    March 2021
    July 2020
    May 2020
    March 2020
    November 2019
    October 2019
    August 2019
    July 2019
    April 2019
    March 2019
    December 2018
    November 2018
    October 2018
    July 2018
    April 2018
    January 2018
    September 2017
    August 2017
    June 2017
    May 2017
    April 2017
    March 2017
    January 2017
    December 2016
    November 2016
    October 2016
    September 2016
    August 2016
    May 2016
    March 2016
    February 2016
    January 2016
    December 2015
    November 2015

    Categories

    All

    RSS Feed